Are humans basically good — kind, generous and peaceful? Or are we essentially evil — cruel, selfish and aggressive? Evolutionary science is uncovering the answer, and it provides important guidance about how we can best live together.
But it should not be oversimplified.
In a recent column on evolution ("Could our real advantage be survival of the friendliest?" Nov. 28), Cass Sunstein discussed the human "self-domestication" theory. Summarizing a recent article by Duke anthropologist Brian Hare and a book by Hare's former teacher, Harvard's Richard Wrangham, Sunstein presents their findings that we — homo sapiens — are a domesticated version of earlier, more aggressive, human species, just as dogs are a docile version of wolves.
Like dogs, we evolved to have lower levels of "reactive aggression" toward those around us, which enabled the cooperation and communication that have propelled our species to world domination.
Sunstein's column was a feel-good piece about how we are hard-wired to get along with each other. That's great and ought to be celebrated.
The story of human domestication is much more interesting than Sunstein let on, however, and it has troublesome implications.
Hare's and Wrangham's answer to the age-old puzzle about human nature is that we have separate neurological pathways for good and evil, and they come to the fore in different contexts and for different reasons. Hitler was kind to his secretary and inconsolable at the death of his dog.
Hare shows that whether people are helpful or hurtful to others depends on how similar the others in question seem to themselves. Instinctive antagonism toward outsiders co-evolved through the same biological mechanisms that molded group solidarity. Think of parents, who are the soul of gentleness with their own children, but would readily kill to protect them.