On my pediatrics rotation in medical school, several residents told me they worked with children in part because they sometimes found themselves judging adults: Did they do drugs? Were they fat? Why did they drink so much?
The idea that Americans should take personal responsibility for their health is not new, but it has recently received renewed attention.
What does it actually mean to take personal responsibility for health?
The basic idea is that if we adopt healthful lifestyles, are compliant patients and save money for our own medical care, we'll feel better, spend less and reduce our burden on others. The details of how this philosophy is applied, however, get complicated: Which programs work and which are counterproductive? Does "personal responsibility" save money or just shift costs from insurers to patients? Who should judge whether we're living a healthy enough lifestyle: Doctors? Insurers? Government?
Vice President Mike Pence has argued for "bringing freedom and individual responsibility back to American health care."
Many Americans think it's OK to ask people with unhealthy lifestyles to pay higher insurance premiums and deductibles. Efforts to inject more personal responsibility into health care, however, have not consistently been shown to lower costs, improve outcomes or save lives. Effectiveness — or lack of it — is often in the eye of the beholder.
That individuals bear some responsibility for their health is undeniable. Behavior contributes to nearly half of cancer deaths in the United States, and up to 40 percent of all deaths. But viewing personal responsibility as a central driver of longer lives and lower medical costs is problematic.
American life expectancy has increased markedly in the past century, and few would argue it's because we now lead healthier lives by dint of willpower. It's also not clear healthful behavior saves money: People who live longer use more medical care. Smoking cessation, for example, may actually increase long-term health care costs. And ensuring that people bear greater financial risk doesn't seem to help them make better decisions: A RAND study found that making people pay more for care does reduce how much they use, but that they cut out both highly effective and marginally effective services.